Ignoring facial emotion expressions does not eliminate their influence on cooperation decisions

  1. Sonia Alguacil 1
  2. Pío Tudela 1
  3. María Ruz 1
  1. 1 Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Spain
Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Año de publicación: 2015

Volumen: 36

Número: 2

Páginas: 309-335

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumen

Pese a que la influencia ejercida por el procesamiento de las emociones ha sido estudiada en diferentes dominios cognitivos, el papel de estas durante la toma decisiones en contextos sociales queda aún por explorar. Utilizamos una tarea de interferencia con el fin de estudiar en qué grado es posible evitar la influencia de las emociones de otras personas cuando nos encontramos en interacción con ellas. Los participantes jugaron, en múltiples rondas, al Juego de la Confianza con ocho compañeros que podían ser de tipo cooperativo o no cooperativo, y cuya expresión facial podía ser de felicidad o de enfado. Las emociones de los compañeros de juego no eran predictivas, en ningún caso, de su grado de cooperación. Tanto en el Experimento 1 como en el Experimento 2 los participantes fueron instruidos de manera explícita que debían ignorar las expresiones emocionales de sus compañeros. La validez de la información personal (el grado de cooperación) fue manipulada entre ambos experimentos. Los Control mechanisms of emotions 331 resultados revelaron un efecto de interferencia emocional; las expresiones de felicidad redujeron el tiempo necesario para tomar la decisión de cooperación, mientras que las expresiones de enfado acortaron las decisiones de no cooperación. Este efecto de interferencia fue replicado en el Experimento 3, en el que la instrucción explícita de ignorar las emociones de los compañeros había sido eliminada. Nuestros resultados muestran que las emociones de otros nos influyen de manera inevitable durante nuestra interacción con ellos. Esta evidencia es coherente con las teorías que defienden la existencia de un estrecho vínculo entre emociones y contexto social.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Augoustinos, M., Walker, I., & Donaghue, N. (2014). Social cognition: An integrated introduction. Sage.
  • Averbeck, B. B., & Duchaine, B. (2009). Integration of social and utilitarian factors in decision making. Emotion, 9, 599-608. http://psycnet.apa.org/DOI/10.1037/a0016509.
  • Barrett, L. F. (2012). Emotions are real. Emotion, 12, 413-429. http://psycnet.apa.org/DOI/10.1037/a0027555.
  • Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Advances in experimental social psychology, 41, 167-218. DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00404-8.
  • Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and economic behavior, 10, 122-142. DOI: 10.1006/game.1995.1027.
  • Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press.
  • Campellone, T. R., & Kring, A. M. (2013). Who do you trust? The impact of facial emotion and behaviour on decision making. Cognition & emotion, 27, 603-620. DOI:10.1080/02699931.2012.726608.
  • Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12, 314-325. DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0612.
  • Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach (pp. 282-309), Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
  • Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature neuroscience, 8, 1611-1618. DOI:10.1038/nn1575.
  • Driver, J. (2001). A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 53-78.
  • Eckel, C. C., & Wilson, R. K. (2003). The human face of game theory: Trust and reciprocity in sequential games. Trust and reciprocity: Interdisciplinary lessons from experimental research, 245-274. http://ssrn.com/abstract=1843493.
  • Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 374-80. DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.001.
  • Egner, T., Etkin, A., Gale, S., & Hirsch, J. (2008). Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional vs. non emotional distracters. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1475–1484. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhm179.
  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
  • Fehr, E., & Camerer, C. F. (2007). Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11, 419-427. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.002.
  • Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2008). Social functions of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 456-468).
  • Fischer, E., & Reuber, A. R. (2011). Social interaction via new social media: (How) can interactions on Twitter affect effectual thinking and behavior?. Journal of business venturing, 26, 1-18. DOI: 10.1016/j.jbusvent.2010.09.002.
  • Forgas, J. P. (1991). Affective influences on partner choice: role of mood in social decisions. Journal of personality and social psychology, 61, 708. DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.61.5.708.
  • Forgas, J. P. (1994). The role of emotion in social judgments: An introductory review and an affect infusion model (AIM). European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 1-24. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2420240102.
  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological bulletin, 117, 39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39.
  • Forgas, J. P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: mood effects on negotiator cognition and bargaining strategies. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74, 565. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.565.
  • Forstmann, B., van den Wildenberg, W., & Ridderinkhof, K. (2008). Neural mechanisms, temporal dynamics, and individual differences in interference control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1854-1865. DOI:10.1162/jocn.2008.20122.
  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought--‐action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 313-332. DOI: 10.1080/02699930441000238.
  • Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. Academic Press.
  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American psychologist, 43, 349. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.5.349.
  • Frijda, N.H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of emotions. In H.R. Markus & S. Kitayama (Eds.), Emotion and Culture (pp.51-87). New York: American Psychological Association.
  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2007). Social cognition in humans. Current Biology, 17, R724-R732. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.068.
  • Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017598.
  • Gaertig, C., Moser, A., Alguacil, S., & Ruz, M. (2012). Social information and economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Frontiers in neuroscience, 6:103. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00103.
  • Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion and emotion regulation. In L.A. Pervin & O.P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 525-552). New York: Guildford.
  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological review, 108, 814-834. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814.
  • Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2007). Feelings and consumer decision making: The appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of consumer psychology, 17, 158-168. DOI:10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70023-2.
  • Ikezawa, S., Corbera, S., & Wexler, B. E. (2013). Emotion self-regulation and empathy depend upon longer stimulus exposure. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, nst148. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst148.
  • Kadosh, K. C., Henson, R. N., Kadosh, R. C., Johnson, M. H., & Dick, F. (2010). Taskdependent activation of face-sensitive cortex: an fMRI adaptation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 903-917. DOI:10.1162/jocn.2009.21224.
  • Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 467-480. DOI:10.1080/026999399379140.
  • Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 505-521. DOI:10.1080/026999399379168.
  • Kopelman, S., Gewurz, I., & Sacharin, V. The Power of Presence: Strategic Response to Displayed Emotions in Negotiations. (2007). Ross School of Business, 1061; IACM 20TH Annual Conference. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.951697.
  • Kopelman, S., Rosette, A. S., & Thompson, L. (2006). The three faces of Eve: Strategic displays of positive, negative, and neutral emotions in negotiations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 81-101. DOI: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.08.003.
  • Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (2009). Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. John Wiley & Sons.
  • LeDoux, J. E. (2001). Emotion circuits in the brain. The Science of Mental Health: Fear and anxiety, 259.
  • Lee, V. K., & Harris, L. T. (2013). How social cognition can inform social decision making. Frontiers in neuroscience, 7:259. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00259.
  • Lelieveld, G. J., Van Dijk, E., Van Beest, I., Steinel, W., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2011). Disappointed in you, angry about your offer: Distinct negative emotions induce concessions via different mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 635-641. DOI:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.015.
  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion,14, 473-493. DOI: 10.1080/026999300402763.
  • Lissek, S., Peters, S., Fuchs, N., Witthaus, H., Nicolas, V., Tegenthoff, M., & Brune, M. (2008). Cooperation and deception recruit different subsets of the theory-of-mind network. PloS ONE, 3, e2023. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002023.
  • Loose, R., Kaufmann, C., Tucha, O., Auer, D. P., & Lange, K. W. (2006). Neural networks of response shifting: influence of task speed and stimulus material. Brain research, 1090, 146-155. DOI:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.039.
  • McCabe, K., Houser, D., Ryan, L., Smith, V., & Trouard, T. (2001). A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11832-11835. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.211415698.
  • Moser, A., Gäertig, C. & Ruz, M. (2014) Social information and personal interests modulate neural activity during economic decision-making. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8:31. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00031.
  • Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 11087-11092. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105.
  • Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. (2004). Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and interpersonal processes. Psychology Press.
  • Parrott, W. G., & Schulkin, J. (1993). Neuropsychology and the cognitive nature of the emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 7, 43-59. DOI:10.1080/02699939308409175.
  • Posner, M. I. (Ed.). (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of attention. Guilford Press.
  • Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: What is magic and what is not. Biological psychology, 92, 492-512. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007.
  • Reeck, C., & Egner, T. (2011). Affective privilege: asymmetric interference by emotional distracters. Frontiers in psychology, 2:232. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00232.
  • Ridderinkhof, R. K. (2002). Micro-and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological research, 66, 312-323. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-002-0104-7.
  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Wijnen, J., & Burle, B. (2004). Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 369–377). New York: Guilford Press.
  • Ruz, M., Madrid, E., Tudela, P. (2013) Interactions between perceived emotions and executive attention in an interpersonal game. Social, Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 8, 838-44. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nss080.
  • Ruz, M. & Moser, A. & Webster, K. (2011) Social expectations bias decision-making in uncertain interpersonal situations. PLoS ONE 6: e15762. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0015762.
  • Ruz, M. & Tudela, P. (2011) Emotional conflict in interpersonal interactions. NeuroImage, 54, 1685-91. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.039.
  • Sanfey, A. G. (2009). Expectations and social decision-making: biasing effects of prior knowledge on Ultimatum responses. Mind & Society, 8, 93-107. DOI: 10.1007/s11299-009-0053-6.
  • Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300, 1755-1758. DOI: 10.1126/science.1082976.
  • Satpute, A. B., Badre, D., & Ochsner, K. N. (2013). Distinct regions of prefrontal cortex are associated with the controlled retrieval and selection of social information. Cerebral Cortex, bhs408. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhs408.
  • Scharlemann, J. P., Eckel, C. C., Kacelnik, A., & Wilson, R. K. (2001). The value of a smile: Game theory with a human face. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 617-640. DOI: 10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00059-9.
  • Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 393-414. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660.
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime, Version 1.1. [Software]. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools. Available from http://www.pstnet.com/support/kb.asp?TopicID=1508.
  • Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others' actions: just like one's own?. Cognition, 88, B11-B21. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00043-X
  • Squire, R. F., Noudoost, B., Schafer, R. J., & Moore, T. (2013). Prefrontal Contributions to Visual Selective Attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 36, 451-466. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150439.
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. DOI:10.1037/h0054651.
  • Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: a model based approach. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 3, 119-127. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsn009.
  • Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308, 1623-1626. DOI: 10.1126/science.1110589.
  • Todorov, A., Said, C. P., Engell, A. D., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends in cognitive sciences, 12, 455-460. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.001.
  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and sociobiology, 11, 375-424. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z
  • Tortosa, M. I., Lupiáñez, J., & Ruz, M. (2013). Race, emotion and trust: An ERP study. Brain research, 1494, 44-55. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.11.037.
  • Tortosa, M. I., Strizhko, T., Capizzi, M., & Ruz, M. (2013). Interpersonal effects of emotion in a multi-round Trust Game. Psicológica, 34, 179-198. http://www.uv.es/psicologica/articulos2.13/3Tortosa.pdf
  • ottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A. & Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry research, 168, 242-249. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.
  • Van Cleemput, K. (2010). “I’ll See You on IM, Text, or Call You”: A Social Network Approach of Adolescents’ Use of Communication Media. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 30, 75-85. doi: 10.1177/0270467610363143.
  • Van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life the emotions as social information (EASI) model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 184-188. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01633.x
  • Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K., & Manstead, A. S. (2004). The interpersonal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. Journal of personality and social psychology, 86, 57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.57.
  • Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2005). Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage, 27, 497-504. DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.042.
  • Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9, 585-594. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011.
  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S., & White, T. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5-13. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5