Disentangling the Effects of Backward/Forward Associative Strength and Theme Identifiability in False Memory
- María Soledad Beato
- Mar Suarez
- Sara Cadavid
ISSN: 0214-9915, 1886-144X
Año de publicación: 2023
Volumen: 35
Número: 2
Páginas: 178-188
Tipo: Artículo
Otras publicaciones en: Psicothema
Resumen
Las memorias falsas se han estudiado ampliamente utilizando el paradigma Deese/Roediger-McDermott. A pesar de la robustez del efecto, existe una amplia variabilidad de resultados que todavía no se comprende completamente. Método: tres experimentos examinaron independientemente el papel de la fuerza asociativa inversa (BAS), fuerza asociativa directa (FAS) e identificabilidad del tema (ID) en el reconocimiento falso (RF). Primero, se manipuló el BAS mientras se controló FAS e ID (Experimento 1). Segundo, se manipuló el FAS mientras se controló BAS e ID (Experimento 2). Finalmente, se manipuló ID mientras se controló BAS y FAS (Experimento 3). Se utilizaron análisis frecuentistas y bayesianos. Resultados: el RF fue mayor en las listas de alto que bajo BAS (Experimento 1), y alto que bajo FAS (Experimento 2). En cambio, el RF fue menor en las listas de alto ID que bajo ID (Experimento 3). Conclusiones: tanto BAS como FAS, variables que promueven procesos de inflación del error, pero también ID, quien promueve procesos de edición del error, contribuyen de forma independiente a la producción de memorias falsas. Aislar el papel de estas variables ayuda a comprender la variabilidad de los falsos recuerdos y a extrapolar las tareas DRM para explorar otros dominios cognitivos
Referencias bibliográficas
- Arndt, J. (2010). The role of memory activation in creating false memories of encoding context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), 66–79. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017394
- Arndt, J. (2012). The influence of forward and backward associative strength on false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 747–756. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026375
- Arndt, J. (2015). The influence of forward and backward associative strength on false memories for encoding context. Memory, 23(7), 1093–1111. https:// doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.959527
- Arndt, J., & Beato, M. S. (2017). The role of language proficiency in producing false memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 95, 146–158. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.03.004
- Arndt, J., & Gould, C. (2006). An examination of two-process theories of false recognition. Memory, 14(7), 814–833. https://doi. org/10.1080/09658210600680749
- Beato, M. S., & Arndt, J. (2014). False recognition production indexes in forward associative strength (FAS) lists with three critical words. Psicothema, 26(4), 457–463. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2014.79
- Beato, M. S., & Arndt, J. (2017). The role of backward associative strength in false recognition of DRM lists with multiple critical words. Psicothema, 29(3), 358–363. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.248
- Beato, M. S., & Arndt, J. (2021a). Questioning the role of forward associative strength in false memories: evidence from Deese-Roediger-McDermott lists with three critical lures. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 724594. https:// doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724594
- Beato, M. S., & Arndt, J. (2021b). The effect of language proficiency and associative strength on false memory. Psychological Research, 85(8), 3134– 3151. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01449-3
- Beato, M. S., & Cadavid, S. (2016). Normative study of theme identifiability: instructions with and without explanation of the false memory effect. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 1252–1265. https://doi.org/10.3758/ s13428-015-0652-6
- Beato, M. S., Cadavid, S., Pulido, R. F., & Pinho, M. S. (2013). No effect of stress on false recognition. Psicothema, 25(1), 25–30. https://doi. org/10.7334/psicothema2012.158
- Beato, M. S., Pulido, R. F., Pinho, M. S., & Gozalo, M. (2013). Reconocimiento falso y ansiedad estado/rasgo [False recognition and state/trait anxiety]. Psicológica, 34(2), 299–311.
- Bodner, G. E., Huff, M. J., Lamontagne, R. W., & Azad, T. (2017). Getting at the source of distinctive encoding effects in the DRM paradigm: evidence from signal-detection measures and source judgments. Memory, 25(5), 647–655. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1205094
- Boldini, A., Beato, M. S., & Cadavid, S. (2013). Modality-match effect in false recognition: an event-related potential study. NeuroReport, 24(3), 108–113. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32835c93e3
- Brainerd, C. J., Chang, M., & Bialer, D. M. (2020). From association to gist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(11), 2106–2127. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000938
- Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 164–169. https://doi. org/10.1111/1467-8721.00192
- Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2005). The Science of False Memory. Oxford University Press.
- Brainerd, C. J., & Wright, R. (2005). Forward association, backward association, and the false-memory illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 554–567. https://doi. org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.554
- Brainerd, C. J., Yang, Y., Reyna, V. F., Howe, M. L., & Mills, B. A. (2008). Semantic processing in “associative” false memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1035–1053. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1035
- Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with some simple guidelines. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 1–38. https://doi. org/10.5334/joc.72
- Cadavid, S., & Beato, M. S. (2016). Memory distortion and its avoidance: an event-related potentials study on false recognition and correct rejection. PLoS ONE, 11(10), Article e0164024. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0164024
- Cadavid, S., & Beato, M. S. (2017). False recognition in DRM lists with low association: a normative study. Psicológica, 38(1), 133–147.
- Cadavid, S., Beato, M. S., & Fernandez, A. (2012). Falso reconocimiento en listas DRM con tres palabras críticas: asociación directa vs. inversa [False recognition in DRM lists with three critical words: forward vs. backward association]. Psicológica, 33(1), 39–58.
- Cadavid, S., Beato, M. S., Suarez, M., & Albuquerque, P. B. (2021). Feelings of contrast at test reduce false memory in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 686390. https://doi. org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.686390
- Carneiro, P., & Fernandez, A. (2013). Retrieval dynamics in false recall: revelations from identifiability manipulations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 488–495. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0361-4
- Carneiro, P., Fernandez, A., & Dias, A. R. (2009). The influence of theme identifiability on false memories: evidence for age-dependent opposite effects. Memory & Cognition, 37(2), 115–129. https://doi.org/10.3758/ MC.37.2.115
- Carneiro, P., Fernandez, A., Díez, E., Garcia-Marques, L., Ramos, T., & Ferreira, M. B. (2012). “Identify-to-reject”: a specific strategy to avoid false memories in the DRM paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 40(2), 252–265. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0152-6
- Carneiro, P., Garcia-Marques, L., Fernandez, A., & Albuquerque, P. B. (2014). Both associative activation and thematic extraction count, but thematic false memories are more easily rejected. Memory, 22(8), 1024–1040. https://doi. org/10.1080/09658211.2013.864680
- Carneiro, P., Ramos, T., Costa, R. S., Garcia-Marques, L., & Albuquerque, P. B. (2011). Identificabilidade dos temas de listas formadas por associação retrógrada (backward): contributo para o estudo das memórias falsas [Theme identifiability of backward associative lists: contribution to false memory research]. Laboratório de Psicologia, 9(1), 23–34. https://doi. org/10.14417/lp.634
- Coane, J. H., McBride, D. M., Huff, M. J., Chang, K., Marsh, E. M., & Smith, K. A. (2021). Manipulations of list type in the DRM paradigm: a review of how structural and conceptual similarity affect false memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 668550. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.668550
- Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(1), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046671
- Del Prete, F., Mirandola, C., Konishi, M., Cornoldi, C., & Ghetti, S. (2014). Paradoxical effects of warning in the production of children’s false memories. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 94–109. https:// doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.721036
- Dienes, Z. (2011). Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: Which side are you on? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 274–290. https://doi. org/10.1177/1745691611406920
- Fam, J., Huff, M. J., Westbrook, R. F., & Holmes, N. M. (2021). The effect of early list manipulations on the DRM illusion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(11), 1924–1934. https://doi. org/10.1177/17470218211012620
- Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 833–848. https://doi.org/10.3758/ MC.38.7.833
- Gallo, D. A., Roberts, M. J., & Seamon, J. G. (1997). Remembering words not presented in lists: Can we avoid creating false memories? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(2), 271–276. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209405
- Gallo, D. A., & Roediger, H. L. (2002). Variability among word lists in eliciting memory illusions: evidence for associative activation and monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(3), 469–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S0749-596X(02)00013-X
- Gallo, D. A., Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (2001). Associative false recognition occurs without strategic criterion shifts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 579–586. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196194
- Gunter, R. W., Bodner, G. E., & Azad, T. (2007). Generation and mnemonic encoding induce a mirror effect in the DRM paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 1083–1092. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193480
- Hautus, M. J., Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2022). Detection Theory: A User's Guide. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003203636
- Howe, M. L., Wimmer, M. C., Gagnon, N., & Plumpton, S. (2009). An associative-activation theory of children’s and adults’ memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(2), 229–251. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.10.002
- Huff, M. J., & Bodner, G. E. (2013). When does memory monitoring succeed versus fail? Comparing item-specific and relational encoding in the DRM paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(4), 1246–1256. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031338
- Huff, M. J., Bodner, G. E., & Fawcett, J. M. (2015). Effects of distinctive encoding on correct and false memory: a meta-analytic review of costs and benefits and their origins in the DRM paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 349–365. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0648-8
- Huff, M. J., Bodner, G. E., & Gretz, M. R. (2020). Reducing false recognition in the Deese-Roediger/McDermott paradigm: Related lures reveal how distinctive encoding improves encoding and monitoring processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 602347. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2020.602347
- JASP Team. (2020). JASP (Version 0.14.1). [Computer software].
- Knott, L. M., Dewhurst, S. A., & Howe, M. L. (2012). What factors underlie associative and categorical memory illusions? The roles of backward associative strength and interitem connectivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 229–239. https://doi. org/10.1037/a0025201
- Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection Theory: A User's Guide. Cambridge University Press.
- Malloggi, S., Conte, F., De Rosa, O., Righi, S., Gronchi, G., Ficca, G., & Giganti, F. (2022). False memories formation is increased in individuals with insomnia. Journal of Sleep Research, 31(3), Article e13527. https:// doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13527
- McCabe, D. P., & Smith, A. D. (2002). The effect of warnings on false memories in young and older adults. Memory and Cognition, 30(7), 1065–1077. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194324
- McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(5), 1177–1194. https://doi. org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.5.1177
- Multhaup, K. S., & Conner, C. A. (2002). The effects of considering nonlist sources on the Deese-Roediger-McDermott memory illusion. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(2), 214–228. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749- 596X(02)00007-4
- Namias, J. M., Huff, M. J., Smith, A., & Maxwell, N. P. (2022). Drawing individual images benefits recognition accuracy in the Deese-RoedigerMcDermott paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(8), 1571–1582. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211056498
- Neuschatz, J. S., Benoit, G. E., & Payne, D. G. (2003). Effective warnings in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott false-memory paradigm: the role of identifiability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(1), 35–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.1.35
- Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., & Patihis, L. (2022). What science tells us about false and repressed memories. Memory, 30(1), 16–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/09 658211.2020.1870699
- Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: an interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(1), 1–75. https://doi. org/10.1016/1041-6080(95)90031-4
- Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803–814. https://doi. org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
- Roediger, H. L., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: a multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 385–407. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196177
- Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2012). E-Prime User's Guide. Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
- Schopen, K., Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., & Muris, P. (2022). Effects of forewarnings on children’s and adults’ spontaneous false memories. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19(2), 177–197. https:// doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2021.1904877
- Suarez, M., & Beato, M. S. (2021). The role of language proficiency in false memory: a mini review. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 659434. https:// doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659434
- Ulatowska, J., Olszewska, J., WiÅniewska, D., & Rogowski, T. (2021). The DRM paradigm in sign language: an investigation of associative memory errors in deaf and hearing signers. Current Psychology, 40(11), 5456–5470. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00492-7
- Wang, J., Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., & Zhou, C. (2019). A self-reference false memory effect in the DRM paradigm: evidence from Eastern and Western samples. Memory & Cognition, 47(1), 76–86. https://doi. org/10.3758/s13421-018-0851-3
- Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Balota, D. A. (2004). Attempting to avoid false memories in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm: assessing the combined influence of practice and warnings in young and old adults. Memory & Cognition, 32(1), 135–141. https://doi. org/10.3758/BF03195826
- Wechsler, D. (2012). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition: Technical and Interpretative Manual. Pearson.